Abstract
The present article analyzes in three studies on undergraduates the relationship between trait curiosity (
Keywords: Curiositycreativitydivergent thinkingcreative self-efficacycreative personal identityself -perceived creative capacity
Introduction
On an intuitive level, it was considered that curiosity is one of the impulses that trigger creativity, and it was defined as the intense desire to explore new and uncertain events (Kashdan et al., 2009) and the motivational desire to act and think in new ways (Kashdan & Silvia, 2009). According to Kashdan and Fincham (2002), “the emotional–motivational state of curiosity is associated with actively acquiring information to create, maintain, and/or resolve meaningful perceptual conflicts or gaps in knowledge” (p. 373);
Problem Statement
However, there are very few studies of the relationship between curiosity and creativity (Hardy, Ness, & Mecca, 2017). A first proof of the relation between curiosity and creativity results from the studies on the association of curiosity with the factor openness to experience in the Big Five Model. Thus, John and Srivastava (1999) used NEO-PI (60 items) and they reported on important correlations between openness and curiosity (r = .51) and the sub-factors of the latter, stretching – the motivation to look for new experiences (r = .50) and embracing – accepting uncertainty (r = .43). Within the theoretical frame of Big Five, curiosity is considered a lower order central facet of the dimension Openness (John & Srivastava, 1999). In the study in which they developed the curiosity measurement instrument, Curiosity and Exploration Inventory-II (CEI-II), Kashdan et al. (2009) found an important relation between curiosity and the dimension Openness to experience. The same study reports on the positive relation between curiosity, stretching and embracing, and another correlate of creativity, namely psychological flexibility, which is defined as the ability to be aware in the present moment and fully in contact with one’s thoughts and feelings without needless defence, and, the situation permitting, persisting or changing behavior in the pursuit of valued aims (Hayes, Luoma, Bond, Masuda, & Lillis, 2006).
Although the work of von Stumm, Hell and Chamoro-Premuzic (2011) does not deal with the relation between curiosity and creativity directly, it concludes that intellectual curiosity is the third pillar of the academic performance in addition to intelligence and conscientiousness. Another piece of evidence related to the association of curiosity with the creative personality comes from the studies carried out by Karwowski (2012, 2014, 2015) which clearly show the relation between curiosity (two of its subcomponents - stretching and embracing), and creative self-concept (CSC) and its parameters: Creative self-efficacy (CSE) – the individual’s belief regarding the capacity to solve a problem which needs creative thinking and functioning, and Creative personal identity (CPI) – the importance of creativity for self-identity, the individual’s belief that he/she is a creative person. Thus, a high CSE is considered to be a characteristic of eminent creators and it contributes to what is called high creativity (Big-C). In the case of low creativity (mini-c), CSE is usually treated as a general domain characteristic strongly related to creative functioning in general, rather than to the personal conviction of being creative in certain domains. The positive effects of CSE may be strengthened by the creative personal identity (
Research Questions
The general research question from which we started: what are the associations between curiosity and creativity measured from different perspectives?
Analysis strategies were conducted using SPSS Version 22 and Amos 20 to examine the descriptive, correlational statistics, and confirmatory factorial analysis.
Purpose of the Study
In the present study, we proposed to examine the pattern of the relation between curiosity and creativity, the latter measured by means of a divergent thinking test and scales of self-assessment of creativity as a creative potential and personality characteristic. We started from the hypothesis that one can obtain substantial correlations between curiosity, its dimensions, and creativity.
Research Methods
Study 1
Participants: We used a sample of 110 undergraduates (37 females) from a large public university (Mage = 18,93; S.D. = 0,66) recruited between 2017–2018
Measures
Results
Table
The comparison with other studies that used the respective scale shows results similar to that of undergraduate samples (Kashdan et al., 2009). In the case of creativity, the standard scores show supra-average values for fluidity, high values for flexibility, and average values for originality.
As one can notice (table
Study 2
Participants: Data for this study comes from undergraduates students from the same public university (N = 110; 52 females; Mage = 19.23; S.D. = 0.87) selected over the period 2017-2018.
Measures
Curiosity was measured with the same instrument as in study 1. For the evaluation of creativity, the test below was used.
The
Results
We obtained significantly moderated correlations between self-perceived creativity (SPCC) and curiosity (r = .34), and its parameters, Stretching (r = .24) and Embracing (r = .33). Therefore, when individuals obtain high curiosity scores, they perceive their possibility of being creative in various situations. Certain authors show that this results from their belief that they will be able to control the situation and to handle the problems (Cervone & Peake, 1986).
Study 3
Participants: We used a sample of 203 undergraduates (60 females) from the same university (Mage = 19,30; S.D. = 1,19) recruited during the didactic activities in the first semester of 2018 academic year.
Measures
Curiosity was measured with the same instrument as in study 1 and 2. Creativity was measured from the perspective of the Creative Self-concept by using the test above.
Findings
The average values obtained in the case of curiosity are relatively similar to those in study 1: M = 34,27 (S.D. = 6,17) for curiosity total score; M = 18,67 (S.D. = 4,22) for Stretching and M = 15,60 (S.D. = 3,22) for Embracing (Table
Correlations between latent higher-order factors of curiosity and creative self-were calculated. Structural equation modeling was used to better deal with the measurement error. To assess model fit, different indexes of fit were examined in confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) using Maximum Likelihood method: chi-square value, df, IFI (Incremental Fit Index), NFI (Normed-of-Fit Index), CFI (Comparative Fit Index), RMSEA (Root Mean Square Error of Approximation). The CFA (bootstrap = 2000) showed good fit: χ² = 255,600; df = 162; χ²/df = 1,57; IFI = .905; NFI = .774; CFI = .903; RMSEA = .055 (table
Per total, the results show a certain overlap between the dimensions of curiosity and creative-self, as well as differences. CSE correlates with both dimensions, Stretching and Embracing, much stronger than CPI does, and CPI is more strongly associated with the level of stretching than with the one of embracing. This suggests that both dimensions of curiosity are important for the belief of the person regarding his/her own creative abilities. The pattern relations between CSE, CPI and Stretching and Embracing is similar to the one obtained by Karwowki (2012) with the observation that in the mentioned study the common base of curiosity and creative self is much more obviously obtained by means of the hierarchical analysis carried out with two higher order correlated latent factors: curiosity and creative self.
Conclusion
Research considers that it is extremely necessary to clarify the role of curiosity in the development of creativity (Kashdan & Fincham, 2002). To this purpose, in the present study there were three sub-studies meant to capture the relation between curiosity and creativity measured by means of a test of divergent thinking, the self-assessment scale, and the scale for the creative self-concept - a surface characteristic of creative personality – with two dimensions, creative self-efficacy, and creative personal identity. There is no relation between curiosity measured with the self-rating scale and creativity measured with a test of divergent thinking. The second study shows that, as curiosity is more and more developed, there is the possibility of getting involved in creative activity. In the third study, one can notice that there are certain overlaps, but also differences between the curiosity trait and its factors, on the one hand, and creative self-efficacy and creative personal identity, on the other hand. CSE correlates with both Stretching and Embracing much stronger than CPI does, while CPI is more strongly associated with the level of stretching than with the one of embracing. The general conclusion one can draw is that for a better understanding of the conditions of the manifestation of creativity it is important to study more variables. For the diagnosis of related psychical variables, such as curiosity, creativity, or divergent thinking, one needs to use a large battery of tests.Like any study, it also has limits. One of the limitations is the sample of students obtained from only one university. In addition to the moderate sample sizes, another limit is the character of the instruments used, the two scales used are of the short scale category with 10 items (CEI-II) and 11 items (SSCS), respectively.
References
- Averill, R. J. (1999). Individual differences in emotional creativity: Structure and correlates. Journal of Personality, 67(2), 342–371.
- Browne, M.W. & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit, In Bollen, K.A. & Long, J.S. (Eds.), Testing structural equation models, Newbury Park, CA, Sage.
- Ceci, M.V., & Kumar, V. K. (2016). A correlational study of creativity, happiness, motivation and stress from creative pursuits, Journal of Happiness Studies, 17(2), 609–626,
- Cervone, D., & Peake, P.K. (1986). Anchoring, efficacy, and action: The influence of judgmental heuristics on self-efficacy judgments and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(3), 492–501.
- Fuchs, G. L., Kumar, V. K., & Porter, J. (2007). Emotional Creativity, alexithymia, and styles of creativity, Creativity Research Journal, 19(2-3), 233–245,
- Gough, H. G. & Heilbrun, A. B. (1980). The Adjective Check List Manual (2nd ed.). Palo Alto, CA, Consulting Psychologists Press.
- Hardy, J. H., Ness, A. M. & Mecca, J. (2017). Outside the box: Epistemic curiosity as a predictor of creative problem solving and creative performance, Personality and Individual Differences,104, 230–237.
- Hayes, S. C., Luoma, J., Bond, F., Masuda, A. & Lillis, J. (2006). Acceptance and commitment therapy: Model, processes, and outcomes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1–25.
- Jaussi, K. S., Randel, A. E. & Dionne, S. D. (2007). I am, I think I can, and I do: The role of personal identity, self-efficacy, and cross-application of experiences in creativity at work. Creativity Research Journal, 19, 247–258.
- John, O. P. & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin& O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 102–138). New York, Guilford.
- Karwowski, M. (2012), Did curiosity kill the cat? Relationship between trait curiosity, creative self-efficacy and creative personal identity. Europe's Journal of Psychology, 8(4), 547–558.
- Karwowski, M. (2014). Creative mindset: Measurement, correlates, consequences. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 8(1), 62-70.
- Karwowski, M. (2015). Development of the Creative Self-Concept, Creativity.Theories – Research – Applications, 2(2), 165–179.
- Karwowski, M., Lebuda, I., & Wiśniewska, E. (2012). Measurement of creative self-efficacy and creative role-identity. High Ability Studies.
- Karwowski, M., Lebuda, I., Wisniewska, E., & Gralewski, J. (2013). Big Five personality factors as the predictors of creative self-efficacy and creative personal identity: Does gender matter? Journal of Creative Behavior, 47(3), 215–232.
- Kashdan, T. B., & Fincham, F. D. (2002). Facilitating Creativity by Regulating Curiosity, American Psychologist, 5, 373–374.
- Kashdan, T. B., & Silvia, P. (2009). Curiosity and interest: the benefits of thriving on novelty and challenge, In C. R. Snyder, S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Oxford Handbook of Positive Psychology, Oxford University Press, (2nd ed.), 367–374.
- Kashdan, T.B., Gallagher, M.W., Silvia, P.J., Winterstein, B.P., Breen, W.E., Terhar, D. & Steger, M.F. (2009). The Curiosity and Exploration Inventory-II: Development, factor structure, and initial psychometrics. Journal of Research in Personality, 43(6), 987–998.
- Kellar, H., Treadwell, H. T., Kumar, V. K., & Leach, S. E. (2002), The Personal Attitude Scale–II: A revised measure of spontaneity. Group Psychotherapy Psychodrama & Sociometry, 55, 35–46.
- Manmiller, J., Kumar, V. K. & Pekala, R. J. (2005). Hypnotizability, creativity styles, absorption, and phenomenological experience during hypnosis. Creativity Research Journal, 17, 9–24.
- Stoica-Constantin, A. & Caluschi, M. (2006). Evaluareacreativităţii. Ghidpractic, [Evaluation of creativity. Practical guide], Iaşi: Editura Performantica.
- von Stumm, S., Hell, B. & Chamoro-Premuzic, T. (2011). The hungry mind: intellectual curiosity is the third pillar of academic performance, Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(6), 574–588.
Copyright information
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.
About this article
Publication Date
15 August 2019
Article Doi
eBook ISBN
978-1-80296-066-2
Publisher
Future Academy
Volume
67
Print ISBN (optional)
-
Edition Number
1st Edition
Pages
1-2235
Subjects
Educational strategies,teacher education, educational policy, organization of education, management of education, teacher training
Cite this article as:
Balgiu*, B. A. (2019). Trait Curiosity And Creativity. Common Bases - Significant Differences. In E. Soare, & C. Langa (Eds.), Education Facing Contemporary World Issues, vol 67. European Proceedings of Social and Behavioural Sciences (pp. 293-300). Future Academy. https://doi.org/10.15405/epsbs.2019.08.03.35