Abstract
The most frequent activities that occur in the context of education internationalization include collaborations, outbound student mobility, and enrolment of overseas students. This resulting student from various origin countries and cultural diversity increasingly occupying classrooms. Numerous cultural concerns may occur when instructors and students from one culture interact with students from another. Furthermore, the fate of China's broad Belt and Road Initiative (BRI), which calls for massive investment in and expansion of trade routes in the area, could have a significant impact on Asian business and the economy. This initiative again, intricately encountered massive cross-cultural issues by all parties involved. This initiative not only affects business people and managers, but also educators and students. Research on the consequences of cultural variations for teaching and learning has consequently been driven by this trend. This study analyses quantitative data comparing China and Malaysia undergraduate students from the perspective of learning behaviour as a group rather than as individual, using Hofstede’s National Culture Model as theoretical background. It is found that applying Hofstede's dimensions directly in exploring culture of learning in the classroom may lead to error in conceptual framework and an act of misuse Hofstede’s work. To generalise these speculations and apply cross-culture pedagogical methods correspond to this thinking may lead to flaw in assumptions and will not positively contribute to theory building as well as ineffectiveness in learning. We need to explore further using systematic conceptualisation and rigorous methods on the assertion of how Hofstede's dimensions impact learning cultures.
Keywords: Cultural dimension, culture of learning, Hofstede
Introduction
What transpires in relation to China's expansive Belt and Road Initiative (BRI), which demands for significant investment in the widening of trade channels in the region, may have a significant impact on business and the economy in Asia. This initiative intricately encountered massive cross-cultural issues by all parties involved. This initiative not only affects business people and managers, but also educators and students. Numerous cultural difficulties may emerge when teachers and students from one culture interact with students from another. Since at least the middle of the 20th century, cross-cultural pedagogy researchers have been alerting instructors and educators about these problems. However, a lot of studies utilised qualitative analysis, and the conclusions of these studies might be hard to relate to one another and occasionally contradict one another. It is still not possible to classify and categorise cultural concerns in the classroom globally, which would enable results of researches to be contextualized and academicians to modify the methodology used. Instead of such a narrow focus, researchers in this discipline typically employ study by Hofstede (1980) in explaining challenges of cultures that occur in learning environment. This study is proposed to analyse quantitative data comparing China and Malaysia undergraduate students in term of Culture of Learning as a group rather than as individual, using Hofstede’s National Culture Model as theoretical background.
This study aims to examine Culture of Learning in two cultures: China and Malaysia. Malaysia and China are not only the two important economies in the One Belt and One Road initiatives, but also encapsulate two distinct cultural traditions. In their widely cited studies, Hofstede (1980, 1986), Hofstede and Hofstede (2001) together with a study in 2010 by Hofstede, Hofstede, and Minkov, identified a number of factors for determining national culture. These dimensions include “individualism vs. collectivism, power distance, uncertainty avoidance, long-term orientation, masculinity vs. femininity, and indulgence vs. restraint”.
National culture
A nation's history, geography, customs, lifestyle, social conventions, and values are all examples of the material and spiritual products that humans have generated, inherited, and evolved. These manifestations are known as its culture. Though many philosophers, socialists, linguists, anthropologists, and others have attempted to describe culture from various angles up to this point, there is hardly any agreement on what culture actually is (Minkov, 2007). To understand the meaning of culture and how it functions in teaching and learning situation, it's certainly essential to appreciate what culture is all about. Anthropologists have debated its definition since at least the 1800s, and academic circles still argue about the term's correct definition and meaning (Minkov et al., 2013). Most authors believed that culture was the culmination of a group of people's aesthetic and intellectual endeavours in the early nineteenth century. Later, the definition was extended by Edward Tylor (1871) by labelling culture as a holistic and complex human endeavour that comprises “knowledge, belief, art, morals, law, custom, and any other capabilities and habits acquired by man as a member of society”. The explanation has since become fundamental concept of culture in anthropology (Spencer-Oatey, 2012). Cultural dimensions describe what further psychological characteristics, such as values, beliefs, self-concepts, personality, and behaviours, vary between groups of culture.
In his book entitled Culture’s Consequences, Hofstede (1980) argues that four dimensions formed the basis for identifying differences in national cultures. A component of a culture that may be assessed in relation to other culture is called a dimension. The main conclusion was that results on the dimensions were significantly correlated with conceptually important external factors (Hofstede, 1991). As a result, a mental health feature from Lynn and Hampson's (1975) study had a correlation with Uncertainty Avoidance scores; Scores for Power Distance include a political systems dimension from Gregg and Banks (1965) and an economic development dimension from Adelman and Morris (1967); Individualism and economic prosperity are corelated (Gross National Product per capita); as well as the amount of national income used for development with Femininity. There were increasing number of external validations reported, and over four hundred (400) substantial relationships between the IBM-based scores and the results of other research are included in the Culture's Consequences by Hofstede and Hofstede (2001), second edition. The disparities between countries that these dimensions represent are, in fact, fundamental and persistent, as evidenced by recent validations that indicate no loss of validity.
On the grounds of Far Eastern-focused study by Michael Harris Bond, psychologist from Canada, a fifth component termed "Long-Term vs. Short-Term Orientation" been inserted in the 1980s (Hofstede & Bond, 1988; Hofstede, 1991; Hofstede & Hofstede, 2001). According to study done in the 2000s using World Values Survey responses and conducted by the Bulgarian scholar Michael Minkov (2007), the sixth dimension was introduced (Hofstede et al., 2010).
Culture of learning
A community or organisational environment that promotes and supports continuous learning and development, growth mindsets, knowledge-sharing, and enhanced performance for both individuals and the business is referred to as having a learning culture. One crucial aspect of culture is that it must be learnt, not inherited (Hofstede, 1991). Language and imitating others fostering education about various cultures. Cultures of learning, on the other hand, is a concept that implies that learning is cultural. Depending on their cultural background, people may have different perceptions and ideas related to teaching and learning (Cortazzi & Jin, 2013). Therefore, to enable learning across cultural populations, it is important to understand learning cultures. People who were raised in a certain cultural community will have an awareness of teaching techniques and classroom norms that correspond to those norms (Charlesworth, 2009). People could have a tendency to take their thoughts on education for granted because these standards are typically unconscious (Li, 2013), without realizing that they are cultural elements rather than an ultimate reality.
Joy and Kolb (2009) mentioned that a thorough framework for comparing learning cultures across nations has not yet been developed and widely incorporated, despite the fact that researchers have tried a variety of methods for distinguishing students based on their cultural backgrounds, from conceptual to practical, (Beckman-Brito, 2003; Parrish & Linder-VanBerschot, 2010). Thus, there is gap in the field.
There were attempts by several researchers to address this gap but more explorative studies are needed before we could clearly understand and settle down with generalised concept of culture of learning applicable across difference national cultures. Parrish and Linder-VanBerschot, (2010) have tried to shed some light on these questions by conceptualising Cultural Dimensions of Learning Framework (CDLF). Eight cultural characteristics related to social interactions, epistemological convictions, and temporal perceptions are described by CDLF, along with examples of their ranges of variability as they could manifest in educational contexts. It was transformed the findings from Hofstede and Hofstede (2005), Nisbett and Masuda (2003), Levine (1997), Hall et al. (2018), and Lewis (2006), and explains how it was manifested in learning situations. The dimensions are (1) “equality and authority”, (2) “individualism and collectivism”, (3) “nurture and challenge”, (4) “stability seeking and uncertainty acceptance”, (5) “logic argumentation and being reasonable”, (6) “causality and complex systems (analysis and holism)”, (7) “clock time and event time”, and (8) “linear time and cyclical time”.
Since Hofstede's model is the one that scholars studying cultures of learning have embraced the most, the model is deployed in this study. Hofstede (1986) and Hofstede et al. (2010) offer a number of suggestions on how the framework might be beneficial for understanding classroom behaviour. In general, according to Minkov and Hofstede (2012), instructional teachers may, “...develop teaching tools using the tables of differences between societies scoring high and low on each dimension” (p. 3).
Power distance
How much people within a community who wield less authority acknowledge and believe the uneven power distribution is referred to as "power distance" (Hofstede et al., 2010). Based on "role pairs" like boss-employee, parent-child, and teacher-student, because the powerful person is perceived as an unquestioned authority in culture with great level of power distance, the power dynamic may turn to authoritarian; The role pairs are more equitable in low power distance societies because the subordinate's involvement is encouraged and expected.
Numerous recommendations regarding how this dimension might appear in the classroom are also made (Hofstede, 1986; Hofstede et al., 2010). In a classroom with high power distance, for instance, students "expect [the] teacher to outline paths to follow," according to Hofstede (1986), whereas those in low power distance classes anticipate having more control over the procedure (p. 313).
Individualism vs. collectivism
Specifically, "Individualism pertains to societies in which the links between people are loose... Collectivism as its opposite pertains to cultures in which individuals from birth onward are incorporated into strong, cohesive in-groups," according to Hofstede et al. (2010, p. 92). People with individualism culture be incline to normalise acting in each individual benefit compared to others as a whole, whereas collective cultures tend to prioritze others before their own personal. Regarding classroom culture, individualism vs. collectivism has also drawn a lot of attention. Collectivism may results students who "may be more tenacious in their efforts to obtain good educational achievements" as well as "may be more likely to accept support from their parents on schoolwork," according to Faitar (2006). Furthermore, in individualism culture, students may feel at ease speaking in front of the entire class and be driven by personal achievement, like accomplishment or extra marks; while students from collectivist cultures are more likely to prefer working in groups and to capitulate; Oyserman and Lee (2008) speculate that a collectivist culture may manifest in the classroom as a reluctance to expose (p. 4).
Uncertainty avoidance
Hofstede et al. (2010) explain: "Uncertainty avoidance can... be described as the degree to which the members of a society feel frightened by ambiguous or unknown situations”. Among its many expressions are anxious tension and a desire for both written and unwritten laws (p. 191). Occasionally, ambiguity tolerance has been used by other writers to describe the opposite of uncertainty avoidance. People who belong to a culture that is highly uncertainty avoidance will avoid vagueness and prioritize timelines, strategies, as well as transparent solutions. Uncertainty can be accepted in cultures with low uncertainty avoidance; for example, ambiguous plans and timelines are acceptable, and a manager not necessarily need to have the right response upon each query to perform a great job. Considering this distinction in the context of societies with high uncertainty avoidance scales might hold onto the idea of an absolute truth, while societies with low uncertainty avoidance scales will typically hold onto a relativistic viewpoint (Hofstede et al., 2010).
Researchers working in classrooms have paid little to no attention to uncertainty avoidance. However, according to Hofstede (1986), students in countries with high levels of uncertainty avoidance like structured learning environments with specific goals, deadlines, and instructions as well as strict teachers who can clearly and precisely answer each student's question. In addition, he asserts that the assertion stated regarding power gap is akin to how educators perceive disagreement as betrayal.
Masculinity vs. femininity
Female has the tendency of being extra submissive, delicate, and prioritize life value, whereas male is described as belligerent, harsh, and motivated by material accomplishment. This is how Hofstede et al. (2010) characterise the dimension of masculinity vs. femininity.
It is stated more explicitly elsewhere by the same authors, mentioned "Masculinity-femininity is about a stress on ego vs a stress on relationship with others, regardless of group ties" (Hofstede et al., 2010, p. 146). Despite using the term "Nurture vs. Challenge" orientation, Parrish and Linder-VanBerschot (2010) describes a concept that is akin to masculinity concept of Hofstede's and contends the causes of the level of rivalry that exists in learning. Hofstede et al. (2010) state that in general, "Masculinity-femininity is about a stress on ego versus a stress on relationship with others, regardless of group ties" (p. 146). According to another article by Hofstede et al. (2010), masculine and feminine cultures have different standards for evaluating teachers and students. The dominating criteria on the masculine side are the academic reputation and brilliance of the teachers, as well as the academic success of the students. On the female side, kids' socialisation and teachers' friendship with them are more important factors. (p. 162) According to Thowfeek and Jaafar's (2012) survey on the topic of blending virtual learning into curricular, "In a feminine culture, accepting a new system will be influenced by others in the organisation, whereas in a masculine culture, decision-making about adopting a new system is influenced by rewards, recognition, training, and improvement of the individuals" (p. 966).
In several ways, Hofstede et al. (2010) make an explicit connection between this dimension and classroom norms. Failure in school is described as "a disaster in a masculine culture... [whereas] failure in school in a feminine culture is a relatively minor incident" (p. 161), "...in the more feminine cultures, the average student is considered the norm, while in more masculine countries, the best students are the norm," (p. 160), and "...weak students are praised in feminine cultures, but only strong students are praised in masculine cultures" (p. 160). Among male student, they have the tendency to apply for taking again the subject when they fails, which would not happen in a feminine culture, according to the researchers, forceful behaviour and attempts at perfection are easily mocked there.
Long-term orientation
Hofstede et al. (2010) elucidated long-term orientation as "the nurturing of virtues directed toward future benefits, particularly perseverance and thrift" by. There is not any explicit analysis of this notion in educational settings. Discussion of Confucianism's influence in the classroom has become more common, despite the fact that it is not the same construct as the one defined by Hofstede and Bond (1988), thus it is worth briefly revisiting. According to Chan (2008), Confucianism, for instance, "encourages the Chinese to respect hierarchical connections between individuals so that the professors are supposed to teach as well as lead students. If students are frequently invited in class to share their thoughts or find a solution on their own, many may believe that inadequate teaching is occurring.
Chan (2008) and Hofstede et al. (2010) both draw a similar conclusion on Confucianism and a tendency for rote learning. However, other researchers dispute this assertion (e.g. Ryan, 2013). The relationship between students in Confucian-Heritage Cultures (CHCs) and group learning is also in dispute. To support their assertion that "Learners from CHC environments prefer working in groups and perform better in groups," Nguyen et al. (2006) mention a number of research (p. 4). Agelasto and Adamson (1998) argues that Chinese and Korean students prefer working alone because they are too competitive to appreciate working in groups. Hofstede et al. (2010) assert that individuals from long- term-focused cultures are more adept at "synthetic" thinking, whereas individuals from short-term-focused cultures are more adept at "analytical" thinking.
Indulgence vs. restraint
According to Hofstede et al. (2010), The likelihood to allow relatively unrestrained gratification of basic and normal human desires related to having fun and enjoying life is known as indulgence. Its opposite, restraint, expresses the conviction that such fulfilment must be restricted and governed by strict social norms.
However, not much has been written about this dimension by other scholars, presumably as a result of its relatively recent use. Hofstede (1986), Hofstede and Hofstede (2001), Hofstede, Hofstede, and Minkov, as well as other authors, do not directly link the newer dimensions to the classroom, in contrast to the earlier ones from Hofstede (1980). This indicates that there aren't any disagreements about this dimension in the literature that need to be settled for teachers just yet.
In order to distinguish between studies of learning cultures that utilise Hofstede's framework legitimately and those that use it improperly, it is necessary to show the empirical reality of his statements. Even though there aren't yet any assertions regarding long-term orientation and indulgence vs. restraint, looking into how these factors relate to learning cultures now may help clear up any ambiguity in the future.
In order to cater the requirements, the following research questions are developed:
RQ1: Do new survey questions which reflect the assertions made by Hofstede (1986) and Hofstede et al. (2010) on learning cultures are internally consistent?
RQ1a: If RQ1 is answered positively, is there any differences between demographic factors and the six dimensions?
RQ2: If Research Questions 1 is answered negatively, can a specific dimensions model for comprehending cultures of learning be produced using a Principal Components Analysis of survey data on items linked to cultures of learning?
RQ2a: If RQ2 is answered positively, is there any differences in demographic factors in relations with the new dimensions?
Research Methods
Participants
This study used empirical survey data of undergraduate students from various disciplines in both Malaysia and China. Since this is an explorative and case research, a convenience sampling is employed. Students from a university in Malaysia (specifically from Universiti Tenaga Nasional) and students from China (specifically from Beijing Institute of Technology, Zhuhai (BITZH) Honors College) were selected to participate in the study. Students from both universities will randomly asked to answer a self- administered questionnaire. There are no Chinese translations of any study documents; they are all written in English. as the present study focusing on students which English as main medium of instruction. Scales for the constructs were tailored for the undergraduate environment and based on prior research when the questionnaire was designed. Every item is scored using a seven-point Likert scale. Along with open-ended questions, the survey also gathered respondent demographic data.
Data collection
Minkov & Hofstede (2012) recommend that 20 participants per country are adequate for a cross- cultural analysis in national culture research; nevertheless, Minkov (2013) notes that 50 has typically been considered as the minimum sample size for cross-cultural survey research. The current study was participated by 157 Chinese and 150 Malaysian.
Description of survey
A printed questionnaire that includes 2 instructional example questions, a 23-item modified version of Minkov & Hofstede (2012) Values Survey Module (VSM), 44 new questions about preferences and behaviours in the classroom, 7 questions about demographics, and one open form for optional feedback makes up the study's instrument.
Content of questions
Questions for this study were developed based on direct adaptation of Minkov & Hofstede’s (2012) VSM for student participants from Malaysia and China. The purpose of this study is to evaluate if Hofstede's assertions regarding how culture affects learning in the classroom can be supported by the analysis of six national cultural variables. Table 1 provides a table of specifications indicating which questions were created for which objective.
Findings
To address Research Question 1, a reliability test was run to check the internal consistency or reliability of the six dimensions. Only internally reliable dimensions are used for further analysis. This study employed a Cronbach’s alpha threshold of 0.6. From Table 1, only one dimension i.e., Indulgence versus Restraint passed the Cronbach’s alpha threshold of 0.6 and considered as having good reliability.
In sum, this study did not provide evidence in favour of generalising at the nation-level regarding preferences and behavioural norms in university classes based on Hofstede's claims about how his dimensions framework connects to them. As a result, it provides a negative response to Research Question 1 and somewhat supports the criticisms made by Signorini et al. (2009) that it is important to question the applicability of Hofstede's theories in the classroom.
To address RQ1a, this study conducted One-Way analysis of variance (ANOVA) for Indulgence versus Restraint by demographic factors which are Origin of Country, Gender, Age, and Year of study. This study did not perform further analysis for the rest of the dimensions as their Cronbach’s alpha values are below threshold of 0.6.
The results of the analysis are shown in Table 2, Table 3, Table 4, and Table 5. The results show that there is significant different between Origin: Malaysian and Chinese, in relation with Indulgence factor. The results also show, there are significant different by Age and Year of study. But there is no significant different of Indulgence versus Restraint with Gender.
Next, a new principle components analysis (PCA) of the complete survey data was carried out to answer Research Question 2 in order to evaluate whether or not Hofstede's model is the most accurate at forecasting national learning cultures features.
The whole set of country mean scores on all items is subjected to principal components analysis (Varimax rotation), which yields six dimensions with Eigenvalues larger than 1. The result shows a KMO value of 0.839 and a significance level for the Bartlett's Test value. These results can be viewed in Table 6. Thus, Research Question 2 is answered positively. Based on the questions of each factor, this study proposes the new conceptual dimensions. Further, this study conducted reliability test for each new factors or dimensions. The results are shown in Table 13. It was found that all factors or dimensions except Factor 3 are reliable.
The first dimension with Cronbach’s alpha of 0.818 is shown in Table 7.
The second dimension with Cronbach’s alpha of 0.613 is shown in Table 8.
The third dimension with Cronbach’s alpha of 0.489 is shown in Table 9.
The fourth dimension with Cronbach’s alpha of 0.749 is shown in Table 10.
The fifth dimension with Cronbach’s alpha of 0.687 is shown in Table 11.
The sixth dimension with Cronbach’s alpha of 0.686 is shown in Table 12.
Based on the questions in Dimension 1, this study suggests elements of convincing, certainty and guaranteed to explain the dimension. Questions in Dimension 2 explain about openness and flexibility, while questions in Dimension 3 promotes individualistic and self-priority. Next, questions in Dimension 4 is related to formalities and questions in Dimension 5 describe on the feeling of being reasonable. Finally, questions in Dimension 6 portrays individual self-determination.
Potential Contributions
This research examines how national culture affects learning styles of undergraduates as a whole not as individual. Theoretically, this research is expected to contribute to our understanding on how national culture affect the way undergraduate students learn and can be a guide for pedagogical approach for learning in different culture especially for comparison between Malaysia and China. This study is predicted to contribute to understanding of economic development in the one road and one belt initiative as to give guidance on how to effectively guide on the cross-culture learning and decision making.
Limitations
This research examines how different culture communities affects culture of learning of undergraduates as a whole not as individual. Theoretically, this research contributes to our understanding on how national culture affect the way undergraduate students learn and can be a guide for pedagogical approach for learning in different culture especially for comparison between Malaysia and China. Nevertheless, the findings from this study should be taken within the limitations of the research. First, generalization of the findings should take into consideration of sample size of the respondents. Since this is an explorative case study research, comparing only students from two universities from two countries; China and Malaysia. For future study and to achieve acceptable generalisation, the sample size should be increase. Second, the choice of students participate in this study are English-medium students and they are exposed to cross-cultural environment. Thus, their National culture dimensions uniqueness may no longer unique to these students. Future research should address this issue as well. Third, the new dimensions of learning culture found by this study need to be tested using quantitative data. The results from this data will be useful to determine whether the new dimensions could serve the purpose of measuring the relationship of national culture and culture of learning.
Conclusions
The findings of this study have broad ramifications, including the following: (a) Published Hofstede ratings might not be immediately relevant for determining culture of learning in modern, English-medium university classrooms per se. This is given the fact that culture of learning is no longer exists in isolation and not unique to certain culture communities. (b) Data from this study also revealed that, with contemporary, English-speaking university students, Hofstede's measurements could not be exactly replicated. The gist of the Hofstede model was created to enhance organisational practices from different cultural communities, and the framework developed were focused to achieve these purposes. The idea of directly using these dimensions to measure culture of learning will not suit the purpose as shown by data from this study. Thus, (c) this study found that, as the principal component analysis result shows, a better model for predicting and measuring national culture influences on cultures of learning do exists. This requires us to further explore the concept and potential for further research.
To conclude, this research has demonstrated that applying Hofstede's dimensions directly in exploring culture of learning in the classroom may lead to error in conceptual framework and an act of misuse Hofstede’s work. We need to explore further using systematic conceptualisation and rigorous methods on the assertion of how Hofstede's dimensions impact learning cultures. To generalise these speculations and apply cross-culture pedagogical methods correspond to this thinking may lead to flaw in assumptions and will not positively contribute to theory building as well as ineffectiveness in learning.
Acknowledgment
This study is funded by Universiti Tenaga Nasional Pocket Grant (J510050002/P202226) and coordinated by Innovation and Research Management Centre (iRMC) of Universiti Tenaga Nasional.
References
Adelman, I., & Morris, C. T. (1967). Society, politics & economic development; a quantitative approach. Johns Hopkins Press.
Agelasto, M., & Adamson, B. (1998). Higher Education in Post-Mao China: An Ecology of Hong Kong (Vol. 1). Hong Kong University Press.
Beckman-Brito, K. (2003). Classroom etiquette: a cross-cultural study of classroom behaviors, Journal of Second Language Acquisition and Teaching, 10, 17-34.
Chan, G. K. Y. (2008). The Relevance and Value of Confucianism in Contemporary Business Ethics. Journal of Business Ethics, 77, 347–360. DOI:
Charlesworth, Z. M. (2009). Cultures of learning: the missing variable, Glion Institute of Higher Education and Les Roches-Gruyère University of Applied Sciences, 5(2), 52-65.
Cortazzi, M., & Jin, L. (2013). Introduction: Researching Cultures of Learning. In M. Cortazzi, & L. Jin (Eds.), Researching Cultures of Learning. Palgrave Macmillan. DOI:
Faitar, G. M. (2006). Individualism versus collectivism in schools. College Quarterly, 9(4), 1-4.
Gregg, P., & Banks, A. (1965). Dimensions of Political Systems: Factor Analysis of a Cross-Polity Survey, American Political Science Review, 59(3), 602-614. DOI:
Hall, W., Schmader, T., Aday, A., Inness, M., & Croft, E. (2018). Climate control: The relationship between social identity threat and cues to an identity-safe culture. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 115(3), 446–467. DOI:
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture's consequences: International differences in work-related values. Sage Publications. DOI:
Hofstede, G. (1986). Cultural differences in teaching and learning, International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 10(3), 301-320. DOI:
Hofstede, G. (1991). Empirical models of cultural differences. In N. Bleichrodt & P. J. D. Drenth (Eds.), Contemporary issues in cross-cultural psychology (pp. 4–20). Swets & Zeitlinger Publishers.
Hofstede, G., & Bond, M. B. (1988). The Confucius connection: From cultural roots to economic growth, Organizational Dynamics, 16(4), 5-21. DOI:
Hofstede, G., & Hofstede, G. J. (2001). Culture's Consequences (2nd Ed.). Sage Books.
Hofstede, G., & Hofstede, G. J. (2005). Cultures and organizations: Software of the mind (2nd Ed.). McGraw-Hill.
Hofstede, G., Hofstede, G. J., & Minkov, M. (2010). Cultures and organizations: Software of the mind (3rd ed.). McGraw-Hill.
Joy, S., & Kolb, D. A. (2009). Are there cultural differences in learning style? International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 33(1), 69-85. DOI:
Levine, L. W. (1997). The opening of the American mind: Canons, culture, and history. Beacon Press.
Lewis, H. R. (2006). Excellence Without a Soul: How a Great University, Forgot Education. Public Affairs.
Li, S. -C. (2013). Exploring the Relationships among Service Quality, Customer Loyalty and Word-Of-Mouth for Private Higher Education in Taiwan. Asia Pacific Management Review, 18(4), 375-389. DOI:
Lynn, R., & Hampson, S. L. (1975). National Differences in Extraversion and Neuroticism, British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. Retrieved July, 1, 2022, from DOI: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1975.tb00175.x
Minkov, M. (2007). What makes us different and similar: A new interpretation of the World Values Survey and other cross-cultural data. Klasika i Stil Publishing House.
Minkov, M. (2013). Cross-cultural analysis: The science and art of comparing the world's modern societies and their cultures. Sage Publications. DOI:
Minkov, M., Blagoev, V., & Hofstede, G. (2013). The Boundaries of Culture: Do Questions About Societal Norms Reveal Cultural Differences? Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 44(7), 1094–1106. DOI:
Minkov, M., & Hofstede, G. (2012). Hofstede’s fifth dimension: New evidence from the World Values Survey. Journal of cross-cultural psychology, 43(1), 3-14. DOI:
Nguyen, P. M., Terlouw, C., & Pilot, A. (2006). Culturally appropriate pedagogy: the case of group learning in a Confucian Heritage Culture context. Intercultural Education, 17(1), 1-19, DOI:
Nisbett, R. E., & Masuda, T. (2003). Culture and point of view. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100(19). DOI:
Oyserman, D., & Lee, S. W. S. (2008). Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects of priming individualism and collectivism. Psychological Bulletin, 134(2), 311–342. DOI:
Parrish, P., & Linder-VanBerschot, J. (2010). Cultural dimensions of learning: Addressing the challenges of multicultural instruction. The International Review of Research in Open and Distributed Learning, 11(2), 1. DOI: 10.19173/irrodl.v11i2.809
Ryan, J. (2013). Comparing Learning Characteristics in Chinese and Anglophone Cultures: Pitfalls and Insights. In M. Cortazzi, & L. Jin (Eds.), Researching Cultures of Learning. Palgrave Macmillan. DOI:
Signorini, P., Wiesemes, R., & Murphy, R. (2009). Developing alternative frameworks for exploring intercultural learning: a critique of Hofstede's cultural difference model. Teaching in Higher Education, 14(3), 253-264. DOI:
Spencer-Oatey, H. (2012). What is culture? A compilation of quotations. GlobalPAD Core Concepts. Retrieved July, 1, 2022, from https://warwick.ac.uk/fac/soc/al/globalpad-rip/openhouse/ interculturalskills_old/core_concept_compilations/global_pad_-_what_is_culture.pdf
Thowfeek, M. H., & Jaafar, A. (2012). Instructors' View about Implementation of E-learning System: An Analysis based on Hofstede's Cultural Dimensions. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, 65, 961-967. DOI:
Tylor, E. B. (1871). Primitive Culture: Researches Into the Development of Mythology. Philosophy, Religion, Art and Custom, 2. https://play.google.com/books/reader?id=RUMBAAAAQAAJ&pg= GBS.PR8&hl=en_GB
Copyright information
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.
About this article
Publication Date
18 August 2023
Article Doi
eBook ISBN
978-1-80296-963-4
Publisher
European Publisher
Volume
1
Print ISBN (optional)
-
Edition Number
1st Edition
Pages
1-1050
Subjects
Multi-disciplinary, Accounting, Finance, Economics, Business Management, Marketing, Entrepreneurship, Social Studies
Cite this article as:
Mohamed Saad, Z., & Sudin, S. (2023). Exploring Culture of Learning Using Hofstede’s Dimensions: Malaysia Versus China. In A. H. Jaaffar, S. Buniamin, N. R. A. Rahman, N. S. Othman, N. Mohammad, S. Kasavan, N. E. A. B. Mohamad, Z. M. Saad, F. A. Ghani, & N. I. N. Redzuan (Eds.), Accelerating Transformation towards Sustainable and Resilient Business: Lessons Learned from the COVID-19 Crisis, vol 1. European Proceedings of Finance and Economics (pp. 942-955). European Publisher. https://doi.org/10.15405/epfe.23081.87